Doing research

Probably every research mathematician has been asked the question, “How do you do mathematical research?” Some lay people think we simply figure out ways of doing bigger and bigger long multiplications. Many more people think that all the mathematics must have been discovered by now, so what are we doing?

These misunderstandings are about what we do research on rather than how we do it, the question I want to focus on here.

In 1945, Jacques Hadamard published a remarkable book, The Psychology of Invention in the Mathematical Field. He was a great mathematician himself, one of whose achievements was the proof of the Prime Number Theorem fifty years earlier. He examined his own mental processes, searched the writings of his predecessors, and sent questionnaires to many leading mathematicians and scientists of his time, including Albert Einstein.

Hadamard’s conclusion was that the process of mathematical discovery can be broken into four parts: preparation, incubation, illumination, and verification. First, one immerses oneself in the problem, thinking about all aspects. Then one takes a break from it, while a mysterious process takes place in the brain, completely below the level of consciousness. Only after this happens is it possible to have a sudden inspiration or breakthrough, the details of which are verified by further work afterwards.

His book was overtaken by the spread of behaviourism in psychology; the questions he was asking were regarded as unscientific, and the methods as too subjective, for a long time. Now that this philosophy has had its day, Hadamard’s question can be re-opened. Indeed, we have techniques for imaging directly the processes occurring in the brain of a mathematician, and further progress should be possible. Hadamard’s book was re-published recently by Princeton University Press under the title The mind of the mathematician.

Here are a couple of great mathematicians quoted by Hadamard, describing how some of their own breakthroughs came about.

Henri Poincaré, in a lecture at Société de Psychologie, Paris, made the following statement, which has been often quoted: by Hadamard; by Robert M. Pirsig, Zen and the Art of Motorcycle Maintenance; by William Byers, How Mathematicians Think; and no doubt by others.

I wanted to represent these [Fuchsian] functions by the quotient of two series; the idea was perfectly conscious and deliberate; the analogy with elliptic functions guided me. I asked myself what properties these series must have if they existed, and succeeded without difficulty in forming the series I have called thetafuchsian.

Just at this time, I left Caen, where I was living, to go on a geologic excursion under the auspices of the School of Mines. The incidents of travel made me forget my mathematical work. Having reached Coutances, we entered an omnibus to go to some place or other. At the moment when I put my foot on the step the idea came to me, without anything in my former thoughts seeming to have paved the way for it, that the transformations I had used to define the Fuchsian functions were identical with those of non-Euclidean geometry. I did not verify the idea; I should not have had time, as, upon taking my seat in the omnibus, I went on with a conversation already commenced, but I felt a perfect certainty. On my return to Caen, for conscience’s sake I verified the result at my leisure.

Then I turned my attention to the study of some arithmetical questions apparently without much success and without a suspicion of any connection with my preceding researches. Disgusted with my failure, I went to spend a few days at the seaside and thought of something else. One morning, walking on the bluff, the idea came to me, with just the same characteristics of brevity, suddenness and immediate certainty, that the arithmetic transformations of indefinite ternary quadratic forms were identical with those of non-Euclidean geometry.

Most striking at first is this appearance of sudden illumination, a manifest sign of long, unconscious prior work. The role of this unconscious work in mathematical invention appears to me incontestible.

The “prince of mathematicians”, Carl Gauss, also quoted by Hadamard, was much more brief and mystical:

Finally, two days ago, I succeeded, not on account of my painful efforts, but by the grace of God. Like a sudden flash of lightning, the riddle happened to be solved. I myself cannot say what was the conducting thread which connected what I previously knew with what made my success possible.

Here is an instance of a mathematician deliberately cultivating the “grace of God”. This is Kathleen Ollerenshaw, who spent her career as a public servant and local politician but never lost her interest in mathematics. The passage is taken from her autobiography To talk of many things, published by Manchester University Press in 1994:

It was at St Leonards, probably because it was a boarding school, that I discovered and developed as a positive habit the powers of what I call “subliminal learning”. We kept very regular hours and were never tired or stressed at school. Lights out at 9p.m., even when in the sixth form. Out of bed at 7a.m. The accepted wisdom is that one should relax before going to bed, emptying the mind of problems. As far as mathematics is concerned I could not agree less. We looked forward to “drawing-room” each evening, but I usually cheated and stole ten minutes back at my desk before bathtime. I would make certain to sort out in my head, as late as possible, what problems needed to be solved the next day and what might be usefully committed to memory. Before falling asleep, I “drew” with my finger any relevant geometrical figure or algebraic equation on the partitioning of the dormitory cubicle that formed a bedside wall. The result would be miraculous. Without fail, on waking in the morning, the details, the logical argument required or the facts that I needed to recall were clearly imprinted in my mind and, because of the clarity, any required solution would often be clearly “written” on the partition. For this to work, it is essential to make sure to wake at least five or ten minutes before the prescribed time for getting out of bed, giving oneself time to go over what has been resolved while asleep. This became honed to a fine art, without my ever telling anyone, and I have used the technique deliberately ever since.

I was interested to see in last week’s New Scientist a brief account of research on players of the computer game Doom. The Brazilian researchers persuaded Doom-players to spend two nights in their lab. Before going to bed on the second night they played the game for an hour. The researchers monitored their dreams by simply waking them during periods of REM sleep and asking them what they had been dreaming about. The next day they played again and the researchers noted their relative performance. Those who had improved most had found elements of the game entering their dreams, but not obsessively. The researchers thought that the subjects had been rehearsing game-playing strategies or solving puzzles in the game while asleep.

Maybe something similar happens in any research …

I remember that, when I was a doctoral student, quite a long time ago, I spent a long time on Hadamard’s first stage, immersing myself in my research problem. When you do this, the problem becomes concrete in various strange and dreamlike ways; and indeed, in my dreams, I found myself wandering round a mysterious landscape which was in some manner the expression of my research problem. I have no doubt that this was the second stage of Hadamard’s process. I can remember the third stage as well. I went into the graduate common room at my college for a cup of coffee. As I picked up the cup, I suddenly saw very clearly what had to be done. As in Poincaré’s account, there was absolutely no need to verify my inspiration then and there. I knew it would work, as indeed it did; it led to my first mathematical publication, in 1969.

About Peter Cameron

I count all the things that need to be counted.
This entry was posted in doing mathematics. Bookmark the permalink.

9 Responses to Doing research

  1. Adam Bohn says:

    Hi Peter,

    More evidence today to back up what you are saying about the link between dreaming and learning:

    http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/health/8638551.stm

    Now we just have to figure out how to get ourselves to dream about our research! Perhaps compulsory beds-in-offices is the way forward?

  2. There are various places in the occult or off-the-wall literature that suggest you can teach yourself how to be more aware of, and exert more control in, your dreams. This is part of Tibetan Buddhist training, and occurs in the Don Juan books by Carlos Castaneda. Probably it works…

  3. I’m not sure whether I should be admitting this or not, but research ideas that have come to me whilst dreaming have a nasty habit of being complete b****cks.

  4. I don’t think that our random remembered dreams will contain good mathematics very often.

    Hadamard, in his book, quotes Poincaré describing a night when he had drunk black coffee and couldn’t sleep: “Ideas rose in crowds; I felt them collide until pairs interlocked, so to speak, making a stable combination”. Poincaré was more aware of his unconscious processes than most of us. (This and his stature as a mathematician suggest that probably this awareness is a good thing).

    On this analogy, the time spent on preparation (thinking about the problem) gets the “blocks” of ideas sorted out so that we know their shape well. The unconscious incubation phase involves this sort of trying out of combinations. Probably some of this happens while we are asleep; the brain forgets the combinations that didn’t work and only remembers the ones that did. (Interesting philosophical question there: can we forget or remember something that never reached our awareness? I would say yes.) The illumination phase then occurs when you sit down to think about the problem, and find that one idea “brings with it” another which fits and advances your understanding. The random dreams would be mostly the combinations that didn’t work, and it is important to the process that these should not be remembered.

    So remembering your dreams may be a good way to train your awareness, but is probably not a good way to do mathematics!

  5. Gil Kalai says:

    Reblogged this on Combinatorics and more and commented:
    To cheer you up in difficult times, here is a wonderful post by Peter Cameron about doing research. And see also this entertaining post by Peter on mathematics and relegion.

  6. Jon Awbrey says:

    Speaking of dreams, the night before last I had a dream where I was listening to a lecturer and something he said made me think of a logical formula having the form ``\text{if if if } a, b, c, d", which I visualized as the Peircean logical graph shown below.

    I knew I had seen something the day before prompting that and a search through my browser history turned up Gil Kalai’s post on Chomskian Linguistics where I had read the phrase “anti anti anti missile missile missile missle”.

  7. I should add that the recent stuff (January 2021) I have posted about graphs on groups has all come by something like Ollerenshaw’s method. Except for the one I put up today, where I woke up with a solution and found that it was not quite right, so I went back to sleep and woke up with the right argument.

  8. Pingback: Animated Logical Graphs • 54 | Inquiry Into Inquiry

  9. A method I have been using very successfully in the past few months is to deliberately go to bed early so that (a) I have plenty of time to do the preparation phase without distractions before going to sleep, (b) I sleep lightly and wake up often to review progress before going back to sleep again, (c) I wake up early and have plenty of time to organise the results and (d) I can then do the verification phase by simply writing everything out directly into LaTeX in more-or-less final form. Then I have the whole day free to do other things. Last night, for example, I went to bed at 8.30 with the problem of trying to sort out why the relations satisfied by the Dirac matrices are wrong, and how to correct them. I woke at 11.30 for an hour’s conscious thought, and again at 3.30 for another hour, and woke up at 6.30 with the solution, written up before breakfast at 7.30.

Leave a comment

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.